Research: News coverage affects perceived blame in traffic crashes

March 6, 2021

by Marcia Hannigan

Have you ever wondered why we say, “a car hit a person” but we don’t say, “a hammer hit a person”?  Assistant professor Kelcie Ralph and Evan Iacobucci ( of the Edward J. Bloustein School of Planning and Public Policy, Rutgers University) worked with colleagues Dr. Calvin  Thigpen (of Lime) and Dr. Tara Goddard (Dept. of Landscape Architecture and Urban Planning, Texas A&M University) to examine the effects of language on how we perceive and respond to traffic crashes.

The full article, “Does news coverage of traffic crashes affect perceived blame and preferred solutions?  Evidence from an experiment” and is available for free via Open Access in the journal Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives.

In previous research, the team reviewed 200 news articles describing car crashes involving people walking (n=100) or biking (n=100).  The authors identified two recurring problems. The first problem occurs at the sentence-level, where seemingly minor choices consistently shift the blame towards the person walking or biking even when the events leading up to the crash were unclear.  For instance, journalists repeatedly used the phrase “a pedestrian was hit” to describe a crash. This sentence commits two sins. First, it assigns agency to an inanimate object. Until self-driving cars are available, this phrase has no place in crash reporting.

To illustrate the awkwardness of this phrase, consider a case where someone gets hit by another person swinging a hammer. The news report would not say, “person hit by hammer.” It would say, “someone hit a person with a hammer.” The second sin is that the sentence makes the person walking or biking the focus of the sentence—the star of the show. Linguistics studies suggest that that the focus of the sentence receives more blame. Another recurring sentence-level problem was the widespread use of the word “accident” instead of “crash”, which further obscures the fact that crashes are preventable.

To determine whether these minor linguistic issues actually matter, the authors conducted an experiment where participants read a news story about a crash and answered questions about who bore responsibility for the crash. 

Readers who encountered standard coverage tended to blame the pedestrian. Some readers encountered a slightly modified description of the crash that used crash, made the driver the focus of sentences, and avoided object-based language. Those readers were much less likely to blame the pedestrian for the crash. 

The experiment also provided evidence about the perverse effect of the second major problem in crash coverage.  Existing coverage tends to treat crashes as isolated events. An alternative approach is to frame crashes thematically by reporting crash statistics and highlighting the common elements in many crashes (high speeds and unsafe road design). By reporting on crashes in isolation, existing coverage obscures the fact that crashes are predictable and preventable. It also reduces the sense of urgency to address crashes because readers have no information about how often crashes lead to serious injuries and deaths.

When readers were exposed to thematically framed articles with crash statistics and a description of the roadway, readers shifted their blame from the pedestrian to features of the roadway. These readers were also more likely to support systems-based solutions to improving road safety like reducing speed limits and changing street design.

This study demonstrates that simple editorial changes can positively affect how readers interpret car crashes. Simple, inexpensive changes can reduce victim-blaming and increase support for evidence-based safety improvements.     

Recent Posts

Laurie Harrington named Executive Dir. of Heldrich Center

Laurie Harrington has been appointed Executive Director of the Heldrich Center for Workforce Development at Rutgers University. She has been serving as Acting Executive Director of the center since February 2024 and previously served as the center’s Assistant Director...

NJSPL – Safely Accommodating Micromobility Innovations

From Lab to Streets: Safely Accommodating Micromobility Innovations By Clinton J. Andrews, Leigh Ann von Hagen, Robert Noland, Hannah Younes, Wenwen Zhang, Jie Gong, Dimitris Metaxas, Desheng Zhang Electric scooters have been widely visible on our streets only...

New Jersey State Policy Lab Celebrates 3rd Anniversary

By Elizabeth Cooner, Ed.D. As we celebrate three years since the inception of the New Jersey State Policy Lab (NJSPL), we are proud of the solid foundation of public policy research we have built. Working with more than 120 faculty members, 80 students, and experts at...

RAISE-24 Recap: Does News Media Spread Fear of AI?

Summary The final round for the RAISE-24 Informatics – Data Science competition was held Friday, April 19, 2024 at the Bloustein School. Hosted by the Master of Public Informatics (MPI) program, the inaugural competition challenge asked competitors “Does News Media...

NJ Unemployment Insurance Claims Dashboard Released

The New Jersey Statewide Data System has released the New Jersey Unemployment Insurance Claims Dashboard. This dashboard uses linked, longitudinal administrative data from the New Jersey Department of Labor and Workforce Development and the New Jersey Office of the...

Upcoming Events

Bloustein School Convocation

Jersey Mike's Arena 83 Rockefeller Road, Piscataway, NJ, United States

The formal BLOUSTEIN SCHOOL CONVOCATION ceremony will recognize each graduate individually with pomp and circumstance.  Students will cross the stage and have their names read as they are recognized. Seating is general […]

Implications of Robotics for Public Policy

Virtual

This presentation offers a systematic analysis of the emerging routes by which applications of embodied artificial intelligence—robotics—elicit public policy responses.