Research: News coverage affects perceived blame in traffic crashes

March 6, 2021

by Marcia Hannigan

Have you ever wondered why we say, “a car hit a person” but we don’t say, “a hammer hit a person”?  Assistant professor Kelcie Ralph and Evan Iacobucci ( of the Edward J. Bloustein School of Planning and Public Policy, Rutgers University) worked with colleagues Dr. Calvin  Thigpen (of Lime) and Dr. Tara Goddard (Dept. of Landscape Architecture and Urban Planning, Texas A&M University) to examine the effects of language on how we perceive and respond to traffic crashes.

The full article, “Does news coverage of traffic crashes affect perceived blame and preferred solutions?  Evidence from an experiment” and is available for free via Open Access in the journal Transportation Research Interdisciplinary Perspectives.

In previous research, the team reviewed 200 news articles describing car crashes involving people walking (n=100) or biking (n=100).  The authors identified two recurring problems. The first problem occurs at the sentence-level, where seemingly minor choices consistently shift the blame towards the person walking or biking even when the events leading up to the crash were unclear.  For instance, journalists repeatedly used the phrase “a pedestrian was hit” to describe a crash. This sentence commits two sins. First, it assigns agency to an inanimate object. Until self-driving cars are available, this phrase has no place in crash reporting.

To illustrate the awkwardness of this phrase, consider a case where someone gets hit by another person swinging a hammer. The news report would not say, “person hit by hammer.” It would say, “someone hit a person with a hammer.” The second sin is that the sentence makes the person walking or biking the focus of the sentence—the star of the show. Linguistics studies suggest that that the focus of the sentence receives more blame. Another recurring sentence-level problem was the widespread use of the word “accident” instead of “crash”, which further obscures the fact that crashes are preventable.

To determine whether these minor linguistic issues actually matter, the authors conducted an experiment where participants read a news story about a crash and answered questions about who bore responsibility for the crash. 

Readers who encountered standard coverage tended to blame the pedestrian. Some readers encountered a slightly modified description of the crash that used crash, made the driver the focus of sentences, and avoided object-based language. Those readers were much less likely to blame the pedestrian for the crash. 

The experiment also provided evidence about the perverse effect of the second major problem in crash coverage.  Existing coverage tends to treat crashes as isolated events. An alternative approach is to frame crashes thematically by reporting crash statistics and highlighting the common elements in many crashes (high speeds and unsafe road design). By reporting on crashes in isolation, existing coverage obscures the fact that crashes are predictable and preventable. It also reduces the sense of urgency to address crashes because readers have no information about how often crashes lead to serious injuries and deaths.

When readers were exposed to thematically framed articles with crash statistics and a description of the roadway, readers shifted their blame from the pedestrian to features of the roadway. These readers were also more likely to support systems-based solutions to improving road safety like reducing speed limits and changing street design.

This study demonstrates that simple editorial changes can positively affect how readers interpret car crashes. Simple, inexpensive changes can reduce victim-blaming and increase support for evidence-based safety improvements.     

Recent Posts

Lindenfeld & Mauri Find Uptick for MOUD at FQHCs

Introduction To reduce morbidity and mortality among individuals with opioid use disorder (OUD), the evidence is strongest for medications for OUD (MOUD), which include methadone, buprenorphine, and naltrexone. Federally qualified health centers (FQHCs) are essential...

NJSPL: Ensuring Accurate & Equitable Vaccination Info

By Yonaira Rivera and Vivek Singh  In today’s digital world, large language models like ChatGPT are important sources of healthcare information, particularly for those who face barriers like insurance issues or language constraints. As part of the IMPACT-NJ project,...

2025 IHC Grant Program Funding Opportunities

The New Jersey Inclusive Healthy Communities (IHC) grant program has announced its latest Request for Proposals (RFP) for grants to local governments and nonprofit organizations to advance policy and systems change through the inclusion of people with disabilities to...

Ralph et al. Review e-Scooter Pilot Projects

Abstract Problem, research strategy, and findings Pilot projects are an increasingly popular tool for dealing with opposition to new ideas. The premise is simple—let experience change minds—but it belies considerable uncertainty. How do opinions evolve during a pilot...

Navigating New Jersey’s Economic Outlook

Navigating New Jersey's Economic Outlook: Insights with Will Irving, Associate Professor of Practice In our first episode of EJB Talks for 2025, Stuart Shapiro and Will Irving discuss his journey from Rutgers MPP student to faculty member and economic forecaster. Will...

Upcoming Events

Event Series CAREERS

Virtual Career Drop-ins

Virtual

Stop by virtually on Mondays (except for holidays) beginning February 3 through April 28 between 11 am and 1 pm to ask a quick (15 min) career-related question of Bloustein […]

Event Series Student Services

Bloustein Librarian Open Office Hours

Bloustein School, Civic Square Building 33 Livingston Avenue, New Brunswick, NJ, United States

Have a research or library question you need assistance with? Visit Open Office Hours with Bloustein Librarian Julia Maxwell. Every Tuesday from 12:00 - 1:00 pm in the Civic Square […]